Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Covariation in the socioeconomic determinants of self rated health and happiness: a multivariate multilevel analysis of individuals and communities in the USA
  1. S V Subramanian,
  2. Daniel Kim,
  3. Ichiro Kawachi
  1. Department of Society, Human Development and Health, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, USA
  1. Correspondence to:
 Dr S V Subramanian
 Department of Society, Human Development and Health, Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Avenue, 7th Floor, Boston, MA 02115, USA; svsubramhsph.harvard.edu

Abstract

Objective: To investigate individual level determinants of self rated health and happiness, as well as the extent of community level covariation in health and happiness.

Design: Multivariate multilevel regression analysis of self rated poor health and unhappiness at level 1, nested within 24 118 people at level 2, nested within 36 communities at level 3. Data were obtained from the 2000 social capital benchmark survey.

Setting: USA communities.

Participants: 24 118 adults.

Main outcome measures: Self reported fair/poor health; and a single item measure of subjective wellbeing.

Results: Controlling for demographic markers, a strong income and education gradient was seen for self rated poor health and unhappiness, with the gradient being stronger for poor health. Community level correlations between self rated poor health and happiness were stronger (0.65) than the individual level correlations (0.16) between the two outcomes.

Conclusion: Poor health and unhappiness are highly positively correlated within individuals, and communities that are healthier tend to be happier and vice versa.

  • subjective wellbeing
  • self rated health
  • happiness
  • multivariate
  • multilevel
  • socioeconomic gradient

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Good health and happiness represent critical aspects of individual welfare, and their pursuit as a worthy end of one’s living has been a subject of countless treatises and, more recently, self help books too. While the quest for happiness and health has re-emerged as a common goal that all individuals and societies are expected to achieve, empirical research on the determinants of health and happiness has remained, for the most part, separate.

As it turns out, health and happiness share important similarities with respect to their determinants. For instance, income poverty has been consistently linked to both unhappiness,1 and poor self rated health.2,3 Social connections (including marriage and friendship) have been robustly linked with both happiness and good health.4,5 Some intriguing differences have also been noted. For instance, health inexorably declines with age, whereas happiness does not. Higher incomes are associated with better health (the so called “gradient in health” that extends well beyond a comfortable standard of living), whereas money, as the popular saying goes, does not necessarily buy happiness.6,7 Moreover, the relation between health and happiness does not necessarily show a tight correspondence. For instance, healthy people are not uniformly happy (presumably because there are other inputs to happiness besides good health). Conversely, people with severe health limitations (for example, paraplegia) have been reported to adjust to their state, so that they can report levels of happiness that are indistinguishable from fully functioning people.8

Despite both health and happiness being end points by themselves, existing research, typically, views them in an unidirectional manner,9 with questions such as: does happiness lead to better health or do states of good health predict states of happiness? In this paper, we bring together two strands of literature on health and happiness within an alternative conceptual framework. Specifically, we consider health and happiness as distinct, yet related, states nested within individuals. To the extent that health and happiness covary across communities, this would provide clues about the potential role of social forces (such as culture, social policies, institutional arrangements) in the codetermination of these two outcomes.

Figure 1 presents the analytical approach adopted for this study. Health and happiness, at level 1, are seen as multiple outcomes nested within individuals, at level 2, who in turn are nested within their local communities, at level 3. Adopting such a multivariate multilevel analytical framework offers two distinct advantages. Firstly, it is only through a multivariate framework that comparable assessments of common individual predictors (demographic and socioeconomic) that affect individual health and happiness can be made. While there have been studies that have investigated in great detail the socioeconomic and demographic patterning in self rated health2 and happiness10 independently, we are not aware of any study that has compared and contrasted how the same individual socioeconomic and demographic markers predict health and happiness, simultaneously. Secondly, the multilevel framework permits an assessment of whether local communities (people’s residential settings) make a difference to individual health and happiness. The combination of a multivariate and multilevel formulation, thus, helps develop the examination of “people-place” relationships in relation to the two dimensions of wellbeing. If higher, contextual, levels are recognised, as shown in figure 1, where individuals are grouped according to the local communities in which they live, then it becomes possible to assess, allowing for each local community’s composition in terms of the demographic and socioeconomic characteristics of individuals within it, whether the proportion of healthy and happy people varies across these communities. There may be substantively interesting differences between the two dimensions, such that local communities that are “healthy” may or may not be “happy”. For instance, according to the world values survey, the Japanese are among the unhappiest people among industrialised countries,11 yet it is also well known that the Japanese have the highest life expectancy in the world.5

Figure 1

 Multivariate multilevel structure of responses (PH, poor health; UH, unhappy) at level 1 nested within individuals at level 2 nested within communities at level 3

There is, therefore, considerable merit to the notion of exploring the covariation of health and happiness, at the individual and community levels. Using a multivariate multilevel analytical approach, the aim of this study was (1) to describe the covariation in health and happiness within individuals, focusing on both their shared determinants, as well as differences in the strength of specific predictors (such as age, sex, race, marital status, and socioeconomic status); and (2) to describe the covariation in health and happiness across communities, focusing on the question of whether healthy communities are also happy communities, after accounting for the individual level relations between demographic and socioeconomic markers and health/happiness.

METHODS

Sources of data

The analysis was based on the 2000 social capital benchmark survey, developed by the Saguaro Seminar at Harvard University. The purpose of this survey was to characterise levels of civic engagement, voluntarism, and social connectedness (or “social capital”) across diverse communities of the USA, and to establish benchmarks for future monitoring.12 Each community sample ranged between 500 and 1500 interviews. In terms of selecting the communities, all community foundations attending the annual meeting of USA community foundations in 1999 were invited to apply. From the communities that applied, 34 local community foundations were selected that broadly represented the diversity of communities across the country (smaller and bigger towns, regions of the country, etc) (T Sander, personal communication, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, 2002). Each sponsoring community organisation then decided what specific area(s) to be surveyed, how many interviews to conduct, and if specific areas or ethnic groups were to be over-sampled. The survey was conducted by telephone by TNS Intersearch using random digit dialling (RDD) within 40 US communities in the year 2000 and in early 2001. Some community foundations operated in more than one community. The participation rates within community samples (that is, the proportion of those eligible who responded to the survey) ranged from a low of 30.2% (Denver, CO) to a high of 57.2% (Newaygo County, MI).12 All survey data were provided by The Roper Center for Public Opinion Research. In most cases, the survey area was a single county or a cluster of contiguous counties. Community samples ranged from municipalities to entire states (Montana, Indiana, New Hampshire, Delaware), suggesting a pronounced variation in the ways communities were identified and defined.

Outcome measures

This study considered self reported responses to single items inquiring about health and happiness.

Self reported health status of individuals was determined by people’s response to a five scale question, “…how would you describe your overall state of health these days? Would say it is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?” Thus, self reported health was defined as the individual’s personal evaluation of their overall health, integrating all aspects of health, without specific reference to any one component of physical, social, mental, or functional health. Public health research has typically used this measure as a proxy for other more “objective”, but often difficult to measure, health outcomes.13 The construct validity of self reported health comes from its demonstrated power to consistently predict mortality or survival in longitudinal studies.14 Meanwhile, the test-retest reliability of self rated health has also been shown to be high, across population subgroups by age and sex.15 We collapsed the categories to form a dichotomous outcome of self rated health: 0 for excellent, very good or good and 1 for fair or poor.

Happiness was assessed by people’s response to the question “In general, are you very happy, happy, not very happy and not happy at all?” As a construct, happiness refers to people’s overall evaluation of their own lives and/or it could capture momentary feelings of pleasure. Because of the multiplicity of meanings that are associated with happiness, it has been referred to as “subjective wellbeing”, even though subjective wellbeing is a more inclusive construct that combines aspects related to “life satisfaction”, “pleasant affect”, and “cognitive evaluations”.9 The happiness measure, used in this study, most probably, relates to “overall happiness” (that is, a general judgment of how life is for that individual) rather than specific components of it such as “hedonic level of affect” (that is, the degree to which various affects that people experience are pleasant in character) or “contentment” (that is, the degree to which a person perceives their aspirations to be met).16,17 Results of the external validation of self report measures of happiness with ratings by friends, family members, and spouses have been consistent,18,19 and the test-retest reliability of subjective wellbeing measures has been shown to be high.20 We dichotomised the four categories of the happiness measure to 0 if very happy or happy; 1 if not very happy or not happy at all.

Thus, we simultaneously modelled the individual probabilities of reporting being unhealthy and unhappy. We assumed that these two measures capture distinct, though related, constructs, in that self rated health and happiness were considered to be valid measures of objective health and subjective wellbeing, respectively. In the final sample, about 12% responded being in poor health while about 5.3% reported not being happy.

Predictors

We considered key individual demographic (age, sex, race, marital status) and socioeconomic (educational attainment, household income) markers as predictors in our analyses. As shown in table 1, except for age, other individual predictors were specified as categorical variables, with a reference category and a set of contrasted dummies. Age was centred about its mean, 45 years. We also considered the effect of local communities on self rated poor health and being unhappy. We did so, not by specifying them as fixed dummy predictors, but by considering them as a level within our analytical framework.21,22 However, four local communities (Montana, Indiana, New Hampshire, and Delaware) in the survey corresponded to entire states. Because our interest was in modelling local community differences in health and happiness, communities that spanned entire states were considered to lack statistically exchangeable properties with the rest of the sample (most of which were defined at the county or metropolitan level). Consequently, we based our third level on 36 local communities (fig 1), and modelled the four “state size” communities as a single fixed indicator variable (referred to as “state community”).

Table 1

 Descriptive information on the individual sample considered for the analytical multilevel models from the 2000 social capital benchmark survey showing the multivariate outcomes, frequency of different individual level demographic and socioeconomic markers, and the three level structure of the sample

Statistical analyses

Multilevel statistical models23 offer an appropriate framework to model the structure illustrated in figure 1—that is, the anticipation that the individual perceptions of health and happiness are nested within individuals who in turn are nested within their local communities. While the concept and application of multilevel statistical models are comparatively well known,21,22,24,25 this study focuses on a comparatively novel extension related to treating multiple outcomes within multilevel models.26 We calibrated a multivariate three level binomial logistic regression that had a structure of 48 236 responses (two for each individual) at level 1 nested within 24 118 individuals at level 2 nested within 36 USA communities at level 3. The important advantage from treating two outcomes together in a multivariate multilevel statistical framework is the estimation of the “covariance” between health and happiness at the community level. When two variables are allowed to be random at the community level not only can we estimate distinct variance terms to summarise the degree to which outcome varies across communities, but we can also estimate a joint covariance to assess the degree and the manner in which they covary across local communities. The fixed and random parameter estimates (along with their standard errors) for the multivariate three level binomial logit link model were calibrated using predictive/penalised quasi-likelihood (PQL) procedures with second order Taylor series expansion,27 as implemented within the MLwiN program.28

Table 2 presents the fixed part results (expressed as odds ratios and their 95% confidence intervals) for individual self rated poor health and unhappiness, modelled simultaneously, as a function of fixed individual demographic and socioeconomic markers and the random effect associated with local communities.

Table 2

 Odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) from the fixed part of a multivariable three level binomial logit model for outcomes, self rated poor health and being unhappy, conditional upon community level random effects

RESULTS

The reference group in table 2 is a 45 year old white married man with some college education and income over $100 000. The probability of reporting poor health and being unhappy for this “most advantaged” socioeconomic group, across all communities except the “state communities”, was 3.4% and 1.5%, respectively.

Age

Age was linearly and positively associated with self rated poor health such that older people were more likely to report poor health as compared with the young. However, there was no statistically significant relation between age and being unhappy.

Sex

No sex differences were seen for self rated poor health suggesting that men and women were equally likely to report poor health. On the other hand, women were about 32% less likely to be unhappy as compared with men.

Marital status

Married people reported being happier and healthier than people who are single, widowed, or separated/divorced. Both unmarried and separated/divorced groups reported higher odds ratios for being unhappy (compared with married groups) than for being unhealthy.

Race

Statistically significant race differentials were seen for self rated poor health as well as for being unhappy with the magnitude being substantially different on the two outcomes. While black people were 25% more likely to report poor health than white people, black people were 51% more likely to report being unhappy. Stronger differences in the magnitude of the effect were also seen for the other category of non-white and non-black race/ethnic group, with this group being 39% and 70% more likely to report poor health and being unhappy, respectively. Thus, the race differentials seemed to be much stronger for happiness than for health.

What this paper adds

  • Individual education and income are strong positive predictors of both self rated health and happiness—that is, money and education do buy both health and happiness.

  • Healthy individuals are likely to be happy individuals, and healthy communities are likely to be happy communities, with the latter covariation being stronger than the former.

Policy implications

There is a need for evaluative assessments of wellbeing to consider the different components that are distinct, yet related, simultaneously, both from an etiological as well as a descriptive standpoint.

Education

We saw a gradient in the relation between educational attainment and self rated health and happiness, with the educational gradient being somewhat stronger for poor health as compared with being unhappy. People with low levels of education were 3.57 times more likely to report poor health while they were only 2.68 times more likely to be unhappy, as compared with people with very high levels of education. People with middle levels of education as well as high levels of education were both more likely to report being in poor health (the former about 1.90 times more likely and the latter 1.63 times more likely). They also were found to have a greater risk of being unhappy with 1.60 and 1.19 times greater odds for the middle and high levels of education, respectively, than for the reference group.

Income

A strong income differential exists for both health and happiness, with the income differential being more or less similar for both the outcomes. Comparing the worst off income category (households making less than $20 000) to the best group (households earning more than $100 000 per year), we found that the worst off were some four times more likely to report poor health and about 3.85 times more likely to report being unhappy. Other income categories also showed significant differentials (compared with the reference group) for both poor health and being unhappy.

Table 3 provides the magnitude of between community variation in the outcomes, poor health and being unhappy, under three different model specifications. The aim was to ascertain to what extent individual socioeconomic and demographic markers account for the between community variation in poor health and being unhappy; and to what extent the community variations in the two outcomes correlated.

Table 3

 Variation and covariation in self rated health and happiness at the individual and community level

Two distinct patterns emerged in the results presented in table 3. Firstly, between community variation was significant even after controlling for individual demographic exposures, and became attenuated only with the inclusion of individual education and income. This would suggest that any clustering of the poor health and being unhappy (by communities) is strongly related to the clustering of individuals (within communities) with respect to their education and income. Consequently, adjusting the model for clustering in individual education and income accounts for the clustering in poor health and being unhappy. Secondly, the patterning of between community variation in poor health seemed to be positively correlated with the patterning of between community variation in being unhappy (correlation coefficient about 0.70). Thus, as shown in figure 2, conditional on individual predictors, communities that are unhealthy are also likely to be unhappy and vice versa. Individual level covariation, however, was relatively less pronounced (correlation coefficient about 0.20), across the three models.

Figure 2

 Scatter plot of the community specific residuals (in logits) for self rated poor health (x axis) and unhappiness (y axis).

DISCUSSION

This paper presented cross sectional evidence on the correlated nature of two components of wellbeing, health and happiness, at the individual as well as the local community level. The key findings of this study are as follows. Firstly, age and sex differentially predict self reported poor health and being unhappy. While age is a strong positive predictor of self rated poor health (that is, older people are more unhealthy), it is a statistically weak negative predictor of happiness (that is, older people are less unhappy). Sex differences were not statistically significant for self rated poor health, but men reported being less happy as compared with women. Secondly, race was a strong predictor of both poor health and being unhappy, but the black/white differential was greater for unhappiness. Thirdly, marital status based differences were stronger for being unhappy than they were for poor health. Fourthly, educational and income gradient ran in the same direction for both the outcomes. We found that the education gradient was stronger for self rated poor health than for being unhappy, with less education leading to greater odds of being unhealthy and unhappy. The income differentials in health and happiness were somewhat less pronounced. In contrast with previous reports that found only a weak relation between income and happiness,29 money does seem to buy happiness.

A novel aspect of our paper was to empirically estimate the covariation in health and happiness at the individual and community levels. While individuals who are unhealthy are likely to be unhappy too, the covariation between self rated poor health and unhappiness was positive and some three times stronger at the community level, even after taking account of individual demographic and socioeconomic factors, than at the individual level.

It may be noted that while individual level demographic and socioeconomic markers accounted for the between community variation in health and happiness, the patterning in the covariation in health and happiness was not explained. This would suggest that while there are common influences on health and happiness, there is also a need to explore a differential and unique set of predictors to account for the individual and local community level covariation in health and happiness.

Besides the well known inferential limits that arise from analysing cross sectional observational data, it is important to add a few caveats regarding the outcome measures applied in this study. Defining and measuring health and happiness is clearly a complex task. For the purpose of the analysis, health and happiness were considered as capturing distinct, though related, constructs (that is, self rated health was viewed as a summary measure for objective health, while happiness was viewed as a critical component of subjective wellbeing). However, given that both are self reported measures there is a potential that the two responses reflect different facets of a common underlying construct such as general physical and mental wellbeing.30,31 Thus, the assessment of self rated health and happiness may have been jointly contaminated by psychological traits such as negative affectivity, or optimism.32 However, some of our empirical findings, particularly the differential predictive power of some of the variables, seem to offset this threat. For instance, self rated health declined with advancing age, whereas happiness did not. Furthermore, the comparatively small individual covariation in health and happiness suggests that it is probably not uncommon for healthy individuals to be unhappy and vice versa. Indeed, it is worth acknowledging that for an evaluative assessment of wellbeing—either for normative purposes or for policy reasons—subjective information, particularly happiness (but also self rated health in poorer communities and societies), may obscure the true extent of inequalities in wellbeing.33,34

Drawing upon multilevel perspectives, this study presented an approach to examining health and happiness as multiple outcomes that simultaneously co-occur and coexist within individuals, with both, additionally, being influenced by the contextual setting of the individuals. Further research, using longitudinal study designs, as well as better specification of community level variables, will be required to explain the potential covariation of health and happiness at the individual as well as at the community levels.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Professor Robert Putnam and Thomas Sander of the Saguaro Seminar at the John F Kennedy School of Government, Harvard University, for their assistance in accessing and encouragement in analysing these data; the other members of the Saguaro Seminar, who helped to design the social capital community benchmark survey; and the participating sponsors and respondents in the 40 communities nationwide represented in these survey results. The paper also benefited from the helpful comments of the five anonymous reviewers.

REFERENCES

View Abstract

Footnotes

  • Funding: none.

  • Conflicts of interest: none.

Linked Articles

  • In this issue
    Carlos Alvarez-Dardet John R Ashton