Anger and psychobiological changes during smoking abstinence and in response to acute stress: Prediction of smoking relapse

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.016Get rights and content

Abstract

Nicotine may be used to manage negative emotions, and recent research suggests that smokers with high levels of hostility may use cigarettes to cope with anger provoking situations. This study evaluated the extent to which a high level of trait anger is associated with risk for relapse among smokers interested in cessation. Chronic smokers with different levels of trait anger provided reports of withdrawal symptoms, craving, and state anger, and collected saliva samples for cortisol during 24-hour ad libitum smoking and the first 24-hour abstinence period of a quit attempt. They also attended a laboratory session conducted after the 24-hour abstinence during which they performed brief mental and social stress challenges and provided blood samples for adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) and cortisol assays. High trait anger was associated with greater increases in state anger, withdrawal symptoms, and craving during the first 24 h of abstinence. It was also associated with greater ACTH concentrations during the laboratory session. High trait anger was also associated with increased risk for early relapse. The findings support the hypothesis that smokers high in anger trait may have greater mood difficulties during abstinence and may be more vulnerable to early relapse than smokers with low anger trait.

Introduction

We examined the extent to which trait anger predicts intensity of craving, withdrawal symptoms, and neuroendocrine changes during the first day of a quit attempt. One of the important reinforcing qualities of smoking is its effects in managing negative affect (Gilbert, 1995, Kassel et al., 2003) and studies have demonstrated that nicotine reduces reports of anger (Jamner et al., 1999). Also, smokers who experience anger situations are likely to increase their smoking behaviors (Jamner et al., 1999, Kalman, 2002, Kenford et al., 2002). However, the extent to which negative affect influences smoking behavior is highly variable between individuals and is influenced by various situational factors (Gilbert, 1995, Abrams et al., 1988, Gilbert et al., 2002, Hughes et al., 1991).

High levels of trait anger may contribute to maladaptive behavioral responses, including a greater likelihood of seeking out substances such as nicotine and alcohol (Whalen et al., 2001, Brady and Sonne, 1999). It is therefore possible that smokers who are high in trait anger draw enhanced benefits from smoking, and may consequently experience more difficulties during abstinence. Consistent with this hypothesis, one study assessed ambulatory anger reports in both smokers and non-smokers who were classified as high or low in hostility (Jamner et al., 1999). Results of that study showed that nicotine administration was associated with reports of reduced anger in people who were high in hostility. Another study showed that response to provocation was reduced in smokers after use of nicotine compared with placebo gum (Cherek et al., 1991). On the other hand, it has been shown that trait hostility predicts higher cigarette smoking rates (Barefoot et al., 1987, Whiteman et al., 1997, Siegler et al., 1992). Also, smokers as a group have been characterized as being higher in neurotic traits, including anger (Gilbert and Gilbert, 1995), and children who are high in anger are prone to take up smoking as college students and be still smoking 20 years later (Lipkus et al., 1994). Considered together, these studies suggest that high levels of anger are likely to contribute to increased tobacco use, and situational factors may accentuate anger-related traits in increasing risk for tobacco use or relapse.

Abstinence from smoking is associated with increased negative affect, including anger and irritability, that begins within 4 to 24 h of abstinence (al'Absi et al., 2002, Piasecki et al., 1998, Swan et al., 1996, Gulliver et al., 1995). The intensity of these symptoms peaks within the first three days (Ward et al., 2001) and may not resolve even after 30 days of abstinence (Gilbert et al., 2002). It is possible that these mood alterations are particularly pronounced among smokers who are high in trait anger. As such, these smokers may also have greater difficulties in their attempt to quit and may relapse earlier than smokers low in anger and hostility. Studies have also shown that enhanced experience of anger is associated with increased physiological responses to acute stress (Bongard et al., 1997, Engebretson et al., 1989). In laboratory studies, effects of anger seem to be more consistently present when provocation of anger is applied (Everson et al., 1995, Engebretson et al., 1989, Larson and Langer, 1997). While the present study does not include a specific anger provocation procedure, it focuses on nicotine withdrawal among smokers, a phenomenon known to increase irritability and anger (Hughes et al., 1991). This natural manipulation may be more relevant to the experience of these individuals than circumscribed laboratory provocation. The study includes multiple measures of mood and hormonal activation to carefully examine these changes during the initial stage of a smoking cessation attempt and their relevance to the risk for relapse.

Smoking relapse rates remain disturbingly high, with approximately 50% of smokers relapsing within 3 days and 75% within the first 2 weeks (Garvey et al., 1992, Law and Tang, 1995, Rose, 1996). Studies have shown that personality traits (e.g., related to depression and anxiety) may also predict risk for smoking relapse (Hall et al., 1993, Hughes et al., 1991, Killen and Fortmann, 1997). Research also suggests that subjects who relapse within the first week of a quit attempt experience greater negative affect than those who successfully abstain (al'Absi et al., 2004). It is therefore possible that individuals prone to increased anger during abstinence are most susceptible to early relapse. Supporting this possibility, a recent study has shown an association between increased trait hostility and risk of early relapse (Kahler et al., 2004). This increased risk of relapse may be related to increased proneness to experience situational irritants, leading to increase in their craving for cigarettes.

The present study addresses the extent to which trait anger is associated with deterioration in mood and hormonal changes during the first day of abstinence and with increased risk of early relapse. Data reported here are part of a larger longitudinal project conducted to examine psychophysiological and biological stress responses, and withdrawal symptoms as markers of relapse (al'Absi et al., 2005).

Section snippets

Participants

This study included 34 women and 38 men who were dependent cigarette smokers between the ages of 18 and 68 (means ± S.E.M. = 37.2 ± 1.68) and who indicated strong motivation to stop smoking. Recruitment was conducted using newspaper advertisements and posters placed in the community. Potential participants were screened first by a telephone interview. The interview included questions concerning any current or recent history of medical or psychiatric disorders, medication intake, and smoking criteria

Subject characteristics

Table 1 shows participant characteristics. Preliminary regression analysis to assess the association between anger and demographic and smoking measures showed no correlation with education, gender, caffeine intake, alcohol use, average hours of sleep, body mass index (ts < 1.6, ps > 0.12). Age was significantly associated with trait anger, with increased age associated with reduced trait anger (t(72) =  2.04, R2 = .06; p < 0.05). Association of trait anger with smoking measures, including age when

Discussion

There are three primary findings relevant to effects of anger on risk for smoking relapse. First, high trait anger was associated with deterioration of mood and withdrawal symptoms during the first 24-hour period of abstinence. Second, high trait anger was associated with attenuated adrenocortical activity during early abstinence, but predicted greater pituitary production of ACTH during the laboratory stress session. Third, both measures of anger (trait and state) predicted shorter time until

References (59)

  • A.J. Garvey et al.

    Predictors of smoking relapse among self-quitters: a report from the Normative Aging Study [published erratum appears in Addict Behav 1992 Sep-Oct;17(5):513]

    Addict. Behav.

    (1992)
  • C. Kirschbaum et al.

    Attenuated cortisol response to psychological stress but not to CRH or ergometry in young habitual smokers

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1993)
  • L.E. Seyler et al.

    The effects of smoking on ACTH and cortisol secretion

    Life Sci.

    (1984)
  • G.E. Swan et al.

    Abstinence effects as predictors of 28-day relapse in smokers

    Addict. Behav.

    (1996)
  • M.M. Ward et al.

    Self-reported abstinence effects in the first month after smoking cessation

    Addict. Behav.

    (2001)
  • M.C. Whiteman et al.

    Hostility, cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in the general population

    Soc. Sci. Med.

    (1997)
  • M. al'Absi et al.

    Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking and mental arithmetic stressors

    Psychophysiology

    (1997)
  • M. al'Absi et al.

    Attenuated adrenocorticotropic responses to psychological stress are associated with early smoking relapse

    Psychopharmacology

    (2005)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (1994)
  • T.B. Baker et al.

    Motivational influences on cigarette smoking

    Annu. Rev. Psychol.

    (2004)
  • J.C. Barefoot et al.

    Suspiciousness, health, and mortality: a follow-up study of 500 older adults

    Psychosom. Med.

    (1987)
  • S. Bongard et al.

    Cardiovascular responses during effortful active coping and acute experience of anger in women

    Psychophysiology

    (1997)
  • K.T. Brady et al.

    The role of stress in alcohol use, alcoholism treatment, and relapse

    Alcohol Res. Health

    (1999)
  • L.L. Brown et al.

    Individual differences in repressive-defensiveness predict basal salivary cortisol levels

    J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.

    (1996)
  • D.R. Cherek et al.

    Human aggressive responding during acute tobacco abstinence: effects of nicotine and placebo gum

    Psychopharmacology (Berl.)

    (1991)
  • L.S. Cox et al.

    Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings

    Nicotine Tob. Res.

    (2001)
  • T.O. Engebretson et al.

    Relations between anger expression and cardiovascular reactivity: reconciling inconsistent findings through a matching hypothesis

    J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.

    (1989)
  • S.A. Everson et al.

    Effect of trait hostility on cardiovascular responses to harassment in young men

    Int. J. Behav. Med.

    (1995)
  • D.G. Gilbert

    Smoking: Individual Differences, Psychopathology, and Emotion

    (1995)
  • Cited by (59)

    • Combining ecological momentary assessment with objective, ambulatory measures of behavior and physiology in substance-use research

      2018, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is also necessary to distinguish between techniques that collect samples in the field for later processing in the laboratory versus live processing. Several types of field collection of biological samples (without field processing) relevant to mHealth studies of substance use have been performed: collection of liquid perspiration to detect opioid and cocaine use in combination with EMA (Linas et al., 2016) or to detect alcohol without EMA (Phillips & McAloon, 1980, but see also Phillips, Little, Hillman, Labbe, & Campbell, 1984); collection of saliva/oral fluid to detect smoking in studies of mobile interventions (Abroms, Boal, Simmens, Mendel, & Windsor, 2014; Free et al., 2011); and collection of saliva/oral fluid for cortisol measurement in smokers and other substance users (al'Absi, Hatsukami, Davis, & Wittmers, 2004; al'Absi, Carr, & Bongard, 2007; Direk, Newson, Hofman, Kirschbaum, & Tiemeier, 2011; Lovallo, Dickensheets, Myers, Thomas, & Nixon, 2000; Sorocco, Lovallo, Vincent, & Collins, 2006; Steptoe & Ussher, 2006; see also Bauer et al., 2011). Although not yet performed with substance users specifically (see al'Absi et al., 2004, 2007 for paper questionnaires completed in the field), to our knowledge, EMA has been successfully combined with the field collection of salivary cortisol in other populations (e.g., Damaske, Zawadzki, & Smyth, 2016; Entringer, Buss, Andersen, Chicz-DeMet, & Wadhwa, 2011; Giesbrecht et al., 2012; Huffziger et al., 2013; Kalpakjian, Farrell, Albright, Chiodo, & Young, 2009; Skoluda et al., 2016; Strahler & Nater, 2017; Van Lenten & Doane, 2016).

    • Anger, anxiety, and depressive affect as predictors of stress-induced cortisol production in khat and tobacco users

      2018, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Hostility has also been found to mediate abstinence-related changes in anger (Quinn et al., 2014). As with other forms of negative affect, trait anger correlates positively with HPA dysregulation indexed via adrenocorticotropic hormone, as well as craving and risk for relapse (al'Absi, Carr, & Bongard, 2007). To our knowledge a simultaneous comparison of the relationship between cortisol and anger, anxiety, and depressive affect has not been examined in the context of concurrent khat and tobacco use.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text